Can Marijuana Make It Better? Prospective Effects of Marijuana and
Temperament on Risk for Anxiety and Depression
Victoria A. Grunberg, Kismet A. Cordova, L. Cinnamon Bidwell, and Tiffany A. Ito
University of Colorado Boulder
Increases in marijuana use in recent years highlight the importance of understanding how marijuana
affects mental health. Of particular relevance is the effect of marijuana use on anxiety and depression
given that marijuana use is highest among late adolescents/early adults, the same age range in which risk
for anxiety and depression is the highest. Here we examine how marijuana use moderates the effects of
temperament on level of anxiety and depression in a prospective design in which baseline marijuana use
and temperament predict anxiety and depression 1 year later. We found that harm avoidance (HA) is
associated with higher anxiety and depression a year later, but only among those low in marijuana use.
Those higher in marijuana use show no relation between HA and symptoms of anxiety and depression.
Marijuana use also moderated the effect of novelty seeking (NS), with symptoms of anxiety and
depression increasing with NS only among those with high marijuana use. NS was unrelated to symptoms
of anxiety and depression among those low in marijuana use. The temperament dimension of reward
dependence was unrelated to anxiety and depression symptoms. Our results suggest that marijuana use
does not have an invariant relationship with anxiety and depression, and that the effects of relatively
stable temperament dimensions can be moderated by other contextual factors.
Keywords: marijuana, harm avoidance, novelty seeking, anxiety, depression
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/adb0000109.supp
Marijuana is the third most commonly used drug in the United
States (after alcohol and tobacco), and the leading illicit drug in
states where its recreational use is currently illegal (CNN Gallup,
2013; National Institute of Drug Abuse, 2014). It is estimated that
more than a third of the American population has used marijuana
and that roughly 7% of Americans currently are regular users
(CNN Gallup, 2013). The perception that marijuana is dangerous
has been decreasing since 2007, corresponding with increasing use
among young people (National Institute of Drug Abuse, 2014) and
increasing legalization for recreational and medical purposes (Colorado Amendment 64, 2012; Washington Initiative 502, 2012).
In the face of such high levels of use and rapid changes to laws
and perceptions, it is critically important to better understand the
consequences of marijuana use. One issue in particular need of
further exploration is the relation of marijuana use to mental
health. Anxiety and depression are the most common mental health
conditions in the United States (National Institute of Mental
Health, 2014b, 2014c), making understanding the factors that
affect them of particular clinical significance. Adding to the clinical relevance, the risk of developing anxiety and depression is
highest within the same age range in which marijuana use is the
highest. That is, 75% of all lifetime cases of anxiety and depression start by age 24 (Kessler et al., 2005) and among adolescents,
roughly 32% have had lifetime prevalence of anxiety disorders and
roughly 14% have had lifetime prevalence of mood disorders
(Merikangas et al., 2010). At the same time, marijuana use is
typically the highest in the teens through early twenties as compared with all other age ranges (Degenhardt et al., 2008; Kessler et
al., 2005), and about 52% of 18- to 25-year-olds have used
marijuana in their lifetime (National Institute of Drug Abuse,
2014).
Here, we take the approach that understanding anxiety and
depression within this population at heightened risk can be improved by examining whether behaviors that are frequent within
this same age range relate to symptoms of anxiety and depression.
That is, given the relatively high rate of marijuana use within late
adolescence/early adulthood and the possibility that it may increase in the face of increasing legalization, there is public health
relevance in knowing the relation of marijuana use to the risk of
anxiety and depression within this age range. This can improve our
understanding of whether increases in marijuana legalization
might affect rates of anxiety and depression, and whether anxiety
and depression prevention and treatment strategies could benefit
by targeting marijuana use.
There is relatively little relevant comorbidity data speaking to
the relation between marijuana and anxiety/depression, as most
large epidemiological studies collapse marijuana use into a broader
Victoria A. Grunberg and Kismet A. Cordova, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder; L. Cinnamon
Bidwell, Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder;
Tiffany A. Ito, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of
Colorado Boulder.
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH)
DA024002 to Tiffany A. Ito and NIH K23DA033302 to L. Cinnamon
Bidwell.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Tiffany
A. Ito, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, 345 UCB, Boulder, CO 80309-0345. E-mail: [email protected]
.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors © 2015 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 29, No. 3, 590 – 602 0893-164X/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/adb0000109
590
substance use disorder category (for review, see Degenhardt, Hall,
& Lynskey, 2003). Those studies that do separately examine
marijuana use focus only on marijuana dependence and/or examine a wide age range (Chen, Wagner, & Anthony, 2002; Degenhardt, Hall, & Lynskey, 2001). Results from studies that have
focused on recreational users and/or young adults are quite variable; some show a negative association between marijuana use and
anxiety/depression (e.g., Denson & Earleywine, 2006; Sethi et al.,
1986; Stewart, Karp, Pihl, & Peterson, 1997), others a positive
association (e.g., Bonn-Miller, Zvolensky, Leen-Feldner, Feldner,
& Yartz, 2005; Hayatbakhsh et al., 2007; Scholes-Balog, Hemphill, Patton, & Toumbourou, 2013), and still others no association
(e.g., Green & Ritter, 2000; Musty & Kaback, 1995). Such a
diverse pattern of results suggests that other factors may also
interact with marijuana use to affect anxiety and depression. Unfortunately, there has been a great deal of diversity in the extant
research along multiple dimensions (e.g., community vs. college
samples, samples unselected vs. selected for marijuana use, different types of marijuana, anxiety, and depression measures),
making it difficult to identify variables that explain the different
patterns of associations obtained. Here we begin the process of
identifying factors that affect the relation between marijuana use
and anxiety and depression by examining a variable that is itself
known to relate to anxiety and depression. We specifically examine relatively stable aspects of temperament whose relation to
anxiety and depression have been frequently studied and ask
whether marijuana use interacts with temperament in its relationship with anxiety and depression.
Temperament, Anxiety, and Depression
According to the biosocial model (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993), temperament affects mental health via genetically
determined biases that influence automatic responses to novelty,
punishment, and reward. The temperament dimension of harm
avoidance (HA) is particularly relevant for understanding anxiety
and depression as it is characterized by heightened apprehension,
shyness, pessimism, and inhibition of behaviors. Given these biases, it is not surprising that HA is positively associated with both
anxiety and depression (Hansenne et al., 1998; Jiang et al., 2003;
Manfredi et al., 2011; Matsudaira & Kitamura, 2006).
While HA likely increases anxiety and depression, marijuana
can have anxiolytic and euphoriant effects. Such positive mood
effects are reported among the top motives for marijuana use (Lee
et al., 2009; Newcomb, Chou, Bentler, & Huba, 1988; Simons,
Correia, Carey, & Borsari, 1998). Animal research suggests a
direct anxiolytic effect of cannabis administration (e.g.,
Guimarães, Chiaretti, Graeff, & Zuardi, 1990; de Paula Soares et
al., 2010; for a review, see Mechoulam, Parker, & Gallily, 2002).
The exact mechanism of these effects has not been determined,
although they seem to be restricted to the effects of cannabidiol
and not 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (e.g., Zuardi, Crippa, Hallak,
Moreira, & Guimarães, 2006) and likely involve serotonergic
receptors in the dorsal periaqueductal gray matter as the basis for
anxiolytic effects (de Paula Soares et al., 2010). Marijuana use
may also facilitate social contact (Green & Ritter, 2000), which
could, in turn, improve mood and ultimately mental health. The
potential for marijuana use to affect mood suggests a possible
moderating role of marijuana on the relation of HA to anxiety and
depression. Specifically, to the degree that marijuana produces
anxiolytic and/or euphoriant effects— either directly through its
biochemical effects on neurotransmitters and receptors or indirectly through expectations and/or the facilitation of mood and
beneficial social interactions—marijuana use may buffer individuals high in HA from increased risk for anxiety and depression.
The other major temperament dimensions in the biosocial model
have shown no consistent associations with anxiety or depression.
Novelty seeking (NS) is thought to bias individuals toward impulsivity and exploration in response to novelty; reward dependence
(RD) reflects a tendency to maintain previously rewarded behaviors. Although these dimensions have been associated with anxiety
and depression in some samples (with occasional negative associations of NS and RD with depression, Farmer et al., 2003; Hansenne et al., 1998), they most often show no association with
anxiety and depression (Copeland, Landry, Stanger, & Hudziak,
2004; Starcevic, Uhlenhuth, Fallon, & Pathak, 1996; Strakowski,
Dunayevich, Keck, & McElroy, 1995; Young et al., 1995). Given
the behavioral biases linked with the temperament dimensions, the
lack of associations with anxiety and depression are theoretically
sensible (i.e., the biases associated with these temperament dimensions would not seem to increase risk for anxiety and depression).
At the same time, these relations have most often been examined
in studies with relatively small samples (fewer than 100 participants), making small effects difficult to detect. More importantly
for the present analyses, the moderating effect of marijuana use has
never to our knowledge been tested.
Current Study
The present study seeks to better understand how marijuana use
relates to anxiety and depression within late adolescents/early
adults by examining how it might moderate the effects of temperament on symptoms of anxiety and depression. We also examine,
in a larger sample than past studies, the relation of temperament to
anxiety and depression. We did this in a prospective design in
which marijuana use and temperament assessed at baseline were
used to predict anxiety and depression symptoms assessed 1 year
later in a relatively large (n 338) sample of 18- to 21-year-old
male and female college students. Roughly equal numbers of men
and women allow us to test whether relations among temperament,
marijuana use, and anxiety/depression differ for men and women.
Hypotheses
Given past research and the nature of the behavioral biases
associated with HA, we predict that baseline HA will positively
predict both anxiety and depression symptoms assessed 1 year
later. However, given potential anxiolytic and euphoriant effects,
we expect marijuana to moderate this relationship, such that the
positive association of HA with anxiety and depression symptoms
will be most evident when marijuana use is low. Marijuana may
itself show a simple relation to anxiety and depression, with fewer
symptoms of anxiety and depression among those who use marijuana more frequently. We assess these relations while also controlling for baseline anxiety and depression. If HA and its interaction with marijuana use have effects independent of current
anxiety and depression, we expect these relations to be evident
even after controlling for baseline levels of psychopathology.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MARIJUANA, TEMPERAMENT, ANXIETY, AND DEPRESSION 591
Given the lack of consistent relations of NS and RD with
anxiety and depression, we had no specific predictions for these
analyses, but we nevertheless tested them to provide a comprehensive assessment of the relations among temperament dimensions, marijuana use, and risk for anxiety and depression.
We have no a priori expectations that these relations will differ
for men and women, but given gender differences in rates of
anxiety and depression (Kessler, McGonagle, Swartz, Blazer, &
Nelson, 1993; McLean, Asnaani, Litz, & Hofmann, 2011), it is
important to assess whether factors that relate to anxiety and
depression differ for men and women.
Method
Participants
Potential participants were recruited via e-mail invitations to
their university account and advertisements on campus to take part
in a 3-year longitudinal study of marijuana use. Those who were
interested in the study were initially interviewed on the phone by
study personnel to determine whether their marijuana use fit into
one of three categories: never users (i.e., never tried marijuana),
relatively infrequent marijuana users (i.e., used marijuana 4 times
or less per month for less than 3 years), and regular frequent
marijuana users (i.e., used marijuana an average of 5 days a week
or more for at least the past year). Both quantity and frequency
criteria were implemented to ensure that variability in marijuana
use reflected relatively stable tendencies. Because the full protocol
also included electroencephalography measures, individuals who
reported a history of head trauma, neurological disorder, or the use
of prescription medication (with the exception of oral contraceptives or medical marijuana) were excluded from the study. One of
the interests in the larger study was on change in marijuana use
over time, so we oversampled participants with lower levels of use,
whom we expected to be more likely to change their use over time.
We continued sampling within each use category until we had
roughly equal numbers of men and women. Participants who met
criteria for inclusion were invited to participate in two sessions a
year for three total years. Data in the present analyses come from
the first sessions in years 1 and 2.
Our final sample consisted of 375 University of Colorado primarily freshman (see Table 1 for sample characteristics). Of the
337 participants who provided racial information, 1 identified as
Black, 12 as Asian, 11 as Hispanic, 1 as Pacific Islander, 2 as East
Indian, 1 as Middle Eastern, 63 as multiracial, and 246 as White.
Four additional participants were initially enrolled but later found
to have provided inaccurate information at the time of recruitment
and so were dropped from the study. Thirty-seven individuals did
not return to complete Year 2, so analyses are based on the 338
with complete data. Those who failed to return in Year 2 did not
differ from those who did in gender, age, race, marijuana use
group, temperament, or psychopathology, all p values .13. Only
the measures of interest to our current hypotheses will be described
Table 1
Sample Characteristics by Marijuana Use Group
Never Infrequent Frequent F or 2 value
Demographics
N (Year 1/Year 2) 126/114 146/133 103/91
Gender (% female) 57 (50%) 72 (54.1%) 47 (51.6%) 0.43
Age 18.30 (0.46) 18.38 (0.52) 18.34 (0.50) 0.76
Ethnicity (% White) 81 (71.1%) 93 (69.9%) 72 (80%) 3.10
Substance use Year 1
Total days of marijuana use (0–30) 0.00a 1.65 (1.88)b 26.07 (3.52)c 4,475.48
Total grams of marijuana use 0.00a 0.87 (1.43)a 24.45 (16.57)b 256.76
Average grams per use day 0.00a 0.32 (0.44)b 0.92 (0.56)c 134.33
Temperament Year 1
Harm avoidance 2.49 (0.66) 2.58 (0.64) 2.52 (0.51) 0.67
Novelty seeking 2.91 (0.42)a 3.08 (0.37)b 3.38 (0.40)c 36.59
Reward dependence 3.55 (0.58) 3.57 (0.53) 3.58 (0.46) 0.10
Psychopathology Year 1
Anxiety symptoms 4.40 (2.60) 4.67 (3.39) 4.91 (3.08) 0.71
Anxiety % at risk 4.39a 15.04b 13.19b 7.79
Depression symptoms 3.64 (2.67)a 4.73 (3.90)b 5.29 (4.00)b 5.81
Depression % at risk 0.88a 8.27b 14.29b 13.53
Psychopathology Year 2
Anxiety symptoms 3.90 (2.79) 4.66 (3.45) 4.40 (2.86) 1.88
Anxiety % subclinical or greater 7.02 11.28 6.59 2.05
Depression symptoms 3.80 (3.28)a 4.99 (4.28)b 4.84 (4.00)ab 3.23
Depression % subclinical or greater 5.26 12.03 10.99 3.62
Note. Gender shows number of females. Ethnicity shows number of Whites. Numbers in parentheses are
standard deviations. Possible ranges are 1–5 for harm avoidance, novelty seeking, and reward dependence; 0 –14
for ASR anxiety symptoms; and 0 –24 for ASR depression symptoms. Higher values indicate greater marijuana
use, HA, NS, RD, anxiety, and depression. Anxiety and depression symptoms reflect total number of symptoms
endorsed. % at risk shows percentage of participants who scored at or above the ASR “at-risk†threshold for clinical
levels of anxiety or depression (T score 65). F and 2 values reflect the test of the omnibus marijuana use group
main effect, df 2, 335, and 2, respectively. Marijuana use group means within the same row with different subscripts
differ at p .05. For omnibus marijuana use group effects, p .05. p .01. p .001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
592 GRUNBERG, CORDOVA, BIDWELL, AND ITO
in detail, but where appropriate (e.g., when they preceded the
measures of interest), other measures collected will be noted.
Self-Report Measures
Temperament and Character Inventory (Year 1 and Year 2).
HA, NS, and RD were measured with the Temperament and
Character Inventory (TCI; Cloninger et al., 1993). HA was assessed with 33 items that assess anticipatory worry, fear of uncertainty, shyness with strangers, and fatigability (e.g., “Usually I am
more worried than most people that something might go wrong in
the future,†“I usually feel tense and worried when I have to do
something new and unfamiliar,†“When I meet a group of strangers, I am more shy than most people,†“I have less energy and get
tired more quickly than most people,†.93). NS was assessed
with 35 items that assess exploratory excitability, impulsivity,
extravagance, and disorderliness (e.g., “When nothing new is
happening, I usually start looking for something that is thrilling or
exciting,†“I often do things based on how I feel at the moment
without thinking about how they were done in the past,†“I often
spend money until I run out of cash or get into debt from using too
much credit,†“I like when people can do whatever they want
without strict rules and regulations,†.85). RD was assessed
with 30 items that assess sentimentality, openness to warm communication versus aloofness, attachment, and dependence (e.g., “I
am strongly moved by sentimental appeals, like when asked to
help crippled children,†“I like other people to know that I really
care about them,†“I like to discuss my experiences and feelings
openly with friends instead of keeping them to myself,†“I don’t
care very much whether other people like me or the way I do
things,†reverse-coded, .88).
All items were answered with respect to how the participants
usually or generally act and feel using a 5-point scale (1
definitely false to 5 definitely true). Separate mean scores were
created for overall HA, NS, and RD, with higher scores reflecting
greater HA, NS, and RD. The biosocial model currently includes
a fourth temperament dimension of persistence (P) that was previously included as part of RD (Cloninger et al., 1993). P is
associated with determination and industriousness. It has been
much less frequently measured in association with anxiety and
depression, and when it has been, shows inconsistent relations
(Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 2006; Hansenne et al., 1999).
Measures of P were omitted from the present study out of space
considerations.
Marijuana use (Year 1). Self-reported marijuana use during
the past 30 days was assessed using the Time-Line Follow Back
(TLFB; Sobell & Sobell, 1992), a calendar-assisted structured
interview in which participants were asked to indicate over the past
30 days the quantity of marijuana used on each day. Frequency and
quantity reports were highly correlated, r .82, p .0001.
Relative to other substances such as alcohol and nicotine, where
individuals might consume an entire beer or cigarette, marijuana
users might just take a few hits. There are also many different ways
to consume marijuana (joints, vaporizers, edibles). Because of this
potential for variability, our main analyses used marijuana use
frequency as our measure of marijuana use. However, secondary
analyses were also conducted with marijuana quantity measures
and yielded identical results (see Supplement Tables S1, S2, and
S3 in the online supplemental material).
Adult Self-Report (ASR; Year 1 and Year 2). Symptoms of
anxiety and depression were measured with the Achenbach System
of Empirically Based Assessment Adult Self-Report (ASR;
Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003), a self-report measure of current
internalizing and externalizing psychopathology that is the adult
parallel to the Child Behavior Checklist. These internalizing and
externalizing scales on the ASR have been well validated and have
adequate psychometric characteristics (Achenbach & Rescorla,
2003). Of interest were anxiety problems and depressive problems.
Participants were asked how well each item described them over
the past 6 months, with responses ranging from 0 not true, 1
somewhat or sometimes true, and 2 very true or often true.
Anxiety problems were assessed with 7 items (e.g., “I am nervous
or tense,†Year 1: .76; Year 2: .77). Depressive problems
were assessed with 14 items (e.g., “I am unhappy, sad, or depressed,†Year 1: .77; Year 2: .80). A total score was
created for each construct with higher scores reflecting a continuous measure of greater endorsement of anxiety and depressive
problems. Because of the conceptual similarity between HA and
anxiety and depression, we conducted factor analyses to confirm
that HA was distinct from anxiety and depression (see online
supplemental material).
Procedure
Participants who met criteria for inclusion were invited to participate in a total of six laboratory sessions over 3 years. Data in
the present analyses come from the first sessions in Years 1 and 2
at which marijuana use, temperament, and psychopathology were
assessed. The assessments occurred approximately 12 months
apart (M 356.98 days, SD 19.78 days).
Participants were instructed to abstain from alcohol for 24 hr,
recreational drugs (including marijuana) for 6 hr, and caffeine and
cigarettes for 1 hr prior to each laboratory session. In both sessions, participants were breathalyzed to ensure a breath alcohol
concentration of zero. Adherence to other abstinence requirements
was verified verbally. Although it would have been preferable to
assess abstinence biochemically, it was prohibitively expensive.
While failure to meet the requirements could add variability to the
responses, none of the participants were visibly impaired, and we
have no reason to think failure to conform to the abstinence
requirements introduced any systematic artifact (i.e., failure to
meet the abstinence requirement seems unlikely to have created
the pattern of relationships among the variables that we observed).
Participants next completed the TLFB followed by a questionnaire
including demographics, the ASR, and the TCI. Prior to the ASR,
participants completed measures of handedness, ADHD symptoms
(Barkley & Murphy, 1998), the Beck Depression Index (Beck,
Steer, & Carbin, 1988), and the Beck Anxiety Index (Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988). Prior to completing the TCI, participants completed the Shortened Self-Regulation Questionnaire
(Carey, Neal, & Collins, 2004). Participants received $25 at each
session.
Analysis Strategy
We first performed preliminary descriptive analyses to assess
the relation between marijuana use, temperament, and anxiety and
depression symptoms, with separate analyses representing mariThis document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MARIJUANA, TEMPERAMENT, ANXIETY, AND DEPRESSION 593
juana use either categorically or continuously. Categorical analyses were done with one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
using a 3-level marijuana use group variable based on the participant’s use at time of study enrollment (i.e., never, infrequent,
frequent). Continuous analyses consisted of bivariate correlations
between number of days marijuana was used in past 30 days from
the baseline Year 1 TLFB and temperament and psychopathology
variables.
Our primary analyses assessed whether Year 1 HA, NS, and RD
predict Year 2 anxiety and depression symptoms, and whether this
relationship is moderated by marijuana use. This was tested using
a cross-lag structural regression approach (Rogosa, 1980). Under
this approach, two multiple regression equations are used to test
the relations shown in Figure 1, illustrated using HA and anxiety
symptoms as an example. Model 1 tests our hypothesized relations
that Year 2 anxiety is predicted by Year 1 HA, and that the relation
between HA and anxiety is moderated by marijuana use. More
specifically, 1 assesses the autoregressive or lagged effect of Year
1 anxiety predicting Year 2 anxiety. Because we assume that initial
anxiety and depression symptoms will be a strong predictor of
subsequent anxiety and depression, including this lagged effect
provides a strong test of the degree to which temperament and
marijuana use predict anxiety and depression over and above
baseline anxiety and depression. Of primary theoretical interest are
2 and 3. 2 tests the effects of Year 1 HA on Year 2 anxiety,
reflecting a simple crossed effect, whereas 3 assesses whether the
simple crossed effect of HA on Year 2 anxiety is moderated by
Year 1 marijuana use. As described by Baron and Kenny (1986),
this predicted moderation is tested by assessing the interaction
between Year 1 HA and Year 1 marijuana use. Although our data
are correlational, given the temporal precedence of the variables
(i.e., that HA and marijuana use in Year 1 are predicting anxiety in
Year 2), significant coefficients for 2 and 3 are consistent with
the possibility that initial HA affects subsequent anxiety, and that
the relation of initial HA on subsequent HA is moderated by initial
marijuana use, respectively (Rogosa, 1980).
To further test our hypothesized relations, additional autoregressive, cross-lagged, and cross-lagged moderation effects are tested
in Model 2 in which Year 2 HA is the outcome. Specifically, 1
assesses the autoregressive or lagged effect of Year 1 HA predicting Year 2 HA. These relations are not of particular theoretical
interest here, but because this tests the temporal stability of temperament, we expect the autoregressive effects in these second
models to be significant. Of primary theoretical interest are 2 and
3. 2 tests the effect of Year 1 anxiety symptoms on Year 2 HA.
Because we expect the relations between temperament and psychopathology to reflect the effect of the more stable temperament
variables affecting subsequent psychopathology rather than initial
symptoms of psychopathology affecting subsequent temperament,
we do not expect initial anxiety symptoms to predict subsequent
HA. Thus, we expect 2 to be nonsignificant. Similarly, we have
no theoretical expectation that the impact of initial anxiety symptoms on subsequent temperament will be moderated by marijuana
use, so we do not expect 3 to be significant. In this way, nonsignificant coefficients for 2 and 3, coupled with significant coefficients for 2 and/or 3, provide additional evidence for our
hypothesized relations. In sum, to the degree that relations between
temperament and anxiety reflect the effect of initial temperament
on subsequent anxiety and not the effect of initial anxiety on
subsequent temperament, we expect significant effects in 2 and/or
3, but not 2 and 3.
This framework just described was repeated 6 times to test the
relation of each aspect of temperament (HA, NS, and RD) on each
outcome variable (symptoms of anxiety and depression). Figure 1
shows a simplified model highlighting the autoregressive, crosslagged, and moderated cross-lagged relations. In addition to these
three variables, each model also contained five additional predictors. First, given the presence of marijuana use in the interaction
term to test for moderation (e.g., Year 1 HA
Year 1 marijuana
use), all models also included the simple effect of Year 1 marijuana use as a predictor. This variable is also of theoretical interest
because it assesses the simple effect of marijuana on anxiety and
depression symptoms. We also included gender and its interactions
with temperament and marijuana use (e.g., HA
Gender, Marijuana Use
Gender, HA
Marijuana Use
Gender) to test
whether interrelations among temperament, marijuana use, and
symptoms of anxiety and depression differ for males and females.
All continuous variables were mean-centered before analyses,
and gender was coded as 1 male and 1 female. All model
assumptions (e.g., homoscedasticity, normality of distributions)
were met. When the predicted interaction between temperament
and marijuana use reflecting our primary test of moderation was
significant, we explored the form of the interaction by plotting and
testing the simple effects of temperament on anxiety or depression
symptoms at lower and higher levels of marijuana use following
Aiken and West (1991). The values of lower and higher marijuana
use selected to test the simple effects were based on examination
of the distribution of marijuana use reported in the TLFB in Year
1 with the goal of assessing effects at values that reflect actual
levels of low and high use in our sample. Based on use within our
sample, we plot and statistically test the effects of temperament on
anxiety and depression at 0 days (low use) and 25.80 days (high
use) of marijuana use, with the latter reflecting the mean level of
use reported by frequent users on the TLFB in Year 1. The low
marijuana use group included all participants recruited as never
users (i.e., they all reported 0 days of use in the past 30 days on the
TLFB) as well as 48 infrequent users who also happened to have
no days of use in the 30 days prior to completion of the Year 1
Figure 1. Sample cross-lag structural regression model. Hypothesized
relations were tested with two multiple regression models. Heavier lines
indicate the two paths of primary theoretical interest assessing the crosslagged effect of initial temperament on subsequent psychopathology (2)
and the degree to which initial marijuana use moderates the cross-lagged
effect of initial temperament on subsequent psychopathology (3).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
594 GRUNBERG, CORDOVA, BIDWELL, AND ITO
TLFB. We also conducted ancillary simple effects tests using .5
standard deviations to represent low (M 2.07 days) and high (M
13.47 days) levels of marijuana use. Results were identical to those
reported here.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics from our sample by categorical marijuana use group classification at time of study enrollment (never, infrequent, frequent). Age, ethnicity, and gender measured in Year 1 did not differ across groups. One-way
ANOVAs on the temperament variables revealed a significant
effect of marijuana use group only on NS, with more frequent
marijuana use associated with higher novelty seeking. One-way
ANOVAs also revealed significant differences in depression
among the marijuana use groups in both Year 1 and Year 2, with
those who use marijuana reporting more depression symptoms.
Marijuana users were also more likely to meet or exceed the
“at-risk†threshold for clinical levels of depression in Year 1 (ASR
T score greater than or equal to 65). While there was no marijuana
use group effect on anxiety symptoms, those who use marijuana
were more likely to meet or exceed the “at-risk†threshold for
clinical levels of anxiety in Year 1.
In addition to examining marijuana use categorically based on
use at study enrollment, we can also examine marijuana use
continuously based on TLFB via bivariate correlations (see Table
2). When examined continuously, marijuana use was only weakly
associated with Year 1 depression symptoms, but not with Year 2
depression symptoms. It was unrelated to anxiety. More frequent
marijuana use was associated with higher NS. Of theoretical interest, HA was positively correlated with Year 1 and Year 2
anxiety and depression symptoms. Neither marijuana use frequency, NS, nor RD were correlated with anxiety or depression
symptoms.
Main Analyses
The preliminary correlational analyses (see Table 2) show consistent simple relations between HA and anxiety and depression
symptoms in both Years 1 and 2. To more specifically test our
hypotheses about the relation of temperament to subsequent anxiety and depression symptoms, as well as the moderating effect of
marijuana on this relation, we conducted a series of cross-lag
regression models, as described in the Analysis Strategy. To facilitate interpretation, Figure 2 presents the coefficients of greatest
interest in testing our hypotheses (cf. Figure 1) while Table 3
presents full model output including all predictors.
Harm avoidance and anxiety. The first model we ran tests
our primary hypotheses that initial temperament predicts subsequent psychopathology, and that this relation may be moderated by marijuana use (i.e., Model 1 in Figure 1). This was done
by regressing Year 2 anxiety on Year 1 HA, Year 1 marijuana
use, Year 1 anxiety, the HA
Marijuana Use interaction term,
gender, and all interaction terms involving gender. This model
revealed three significant effects (see Table 3). Not surprisingly, Year 1 anxiety symptoms significantly predicted Year 2
anxiety symptoms. There was also a significant gender effect,
with women reporting more Year 2 anxiety symptoms than
men. Of interest, when these other variables were included in
the model, HA was not an independent predictor of Year 2
anxiety symptoms. However, consistent with hypotheses, HA
did interact with marijuana use in predicting anxiety symptoms
(Figure 2, Panel A). To understand this interaction, we conducted simple slope analyses separately assessing the relation
between HA and Year 2 anxiety for those low and high in
marijuana use (Aiken & West, 1991). As can be seen in Figure
3, Panel A, when frequency of marijuana use was low, increases
in Year 1 HA were associated with greater anxiety in Year 2,
.15, t(329) 2.69, p .01. By contrast, as predicted,
marijuana use had a buffering effect as reflected in a nonsignificant relation between HA and anxiety when marijuana use
frequency was high, .14, t(329) 1.40, p .16.
To further evaluate our hypotheses, we also tested a second
model assessing the other possible cross-lagged effect—that initial
psychopathology predicts subsequent temperament (Model 2 in
Figure 1). This was tested by regressing Year 2 HA on Year 1 HA,
Year 1 anxiety, Year 1 marijuana use, the Anxiety
Marijuana
Use interaction term, gender, and all interaction terms involving
gender. Not surprisingly, Year 1 HA predicted Year 2 HA (see
Table 3). Of primary theoretical relevance, neither the simple
cross-lagged effect of Year 1 anxiety nor the moderated crosslagged effect of Year 1 Anxiety
Marijuana Use were significant
(Figure 2, Panel A). The only other significant effect in this model
was the Anxiety
Gender interaction. Tests of simple slopes
showed that Year 1 anxiety was associated with greater Year 2 HA
for women, .11, t(329) 2.15, p .03, but not men,
.04, t(329) .79, p .43.
Table 2
Bivariate Correlations Among Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Year 1 marijuana use —
2. Harm avoidance .01 —
3. Novelty seeking .40 .32 —
4. Reward dependence .01 .14 .12 —
5. Year 1 anxiety .05 .57 .06 .03 —
6. Year 1 depression .13 .55 .04 .02 .68 —
7. Year 2 anxiety .01 .46 .04 .00 .68 .50 —
8. Year 2 depression .05 .42 .05 .06 .51 .63 .69
p .10. p .05. p .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MARIJUANA, TEMPERAMENT, ANXIETY, AND DEPRESSION 595
Harm avoidance and depression. Figure 2, Panel B shows
the relations of primary theoretical interest in the cross-lag
model assessing the relation between HA and depression symptoms. Considering the first regression model that tests whether
initial temperament predicts subsequent psychopathology, Year
1 depression symptoms were a significant predictor of Year 2
depression symptoms (see Table 3). The only other significant
effect was the predicted HA
Marijuana Use interaction.
Simple effects tests revealed a pattern of effects very similar to
that obtained for anxiety (Figure 3, Panel B): HA was significantly positively associated with depression symptoms when
marijuana use frequency was low, .15, t(329) 2.69, p
.01, but marijuana use appeared to have a buffering effect as reflected
in a nonsignificant negative relation between HA and depression
symptoms at high levels of marijuana use frequency, .09,
t(329) .92, p .36.
In the second model predicting Year 2 HA, the only significant
predictor was Year 1 HA (see Table 3). Of importance, Year 1
depression symptoms did not predict Year 2 HA, nor did the Year
1 Depression
Marijuana Use interaction.
Novelty seeking and anxiety. The cross-lag model in Figure
2, Panel C shows the coefficients of primary theoretical interest in
assessing the relation between NS and anxiety. Considering the
first regression model that tests whether initial temperament predicts subsequent psychopathology, three effects were significant.
Year 1 anxiety symptoms significantly predicted Year 2 anxiety
symptoms, and greater marijuana use frequency in Year 1 was
associated with less anxiety in Year 2 (see Table 3). While NS did
not have a direct effect on anxiety levels, its effect was moderated
by marijuana use, as reflected in the NS
Marijuana Use interaction. Simple effects displayed in Figure 3, Panel C show that the
relation between NS and anxiety occurs among those with high
marijuana use frequency. That is, when marijuana use frequency
was high, Year 1 NS was positively associated with anxiety
symptoms, .28, t(329) 3.46, p .001. There was no
relation between NS and anxiety symptoms when marijuana use
frequency was low, .08, t(329) 1.61, p .11.
In the second model predicting Year 2 NS, the only significant
predictor was Year 1 NS (see Table 3). Year 1 anxiety did not
predict Year 2 NS, nor did the Year 1 Anxiety
Marijuana Use
interaction.
Novelty seeking and depression. The regression models predicting Year 2 depression from NS revealed effects very similar to
those in the models predicting anxiety from NS (See Figure 2,
Panel D). In the first regression model that tests whether initial
temperament predicts subsequent psychopathology, there were
Figure 2. Cross-lag structural regression models assessing the relation of harm avoidance and marijuana use
to levels of anxiety (Panel A) and depression (Panel B), novelty seeking and marijuana use to levels of anxiety
(Panel C) and depression (Panel D), and reward dependence and marijuana use to levels of anxiety (Panel E) and
depression (Panel F). Heavier lines indicate the cross-lagged and moderated cross-lagged relations of primary
theoretical significance (see Table 3 for exact significance levels). p .05.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
596 GRUNBERG, CORDOVA, BIDWELL, AND ITO
Table 3
Full Regression Models
Harm avoidance
HA and anxiety HA and depression
Model 1: DV Anxiety t p Model 1: DV Depression t p
Year 1 anxiety 0.62 12.76 .001 Year 1 depression 0.59 11.30 .001
Year 1 HA 0.06 1.24 .22 Year 1 HA 0.08 1.52 .13
Marijuana use frequency 0.02 0.54 .59 Marijuana use frequency 0.03 0.65 .52
Gender 0.09 2.31 .02 Gender 0.01 0.17 .87
HA
Marijuana Use 0.11 2.69 .01 HA
Marijuana Use 0.10 2.22 .03
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.01 0.20 .84 Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.50 .62
HA
Gender 0.00 0.09 .93 HA
Gender 0.00 0.07 .94
HA
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.35 .73 HA
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.49 .63
Model 2: DV HA t p Model 2: DV HA t p
Year 1 HA 0.76 18.88 .001 Year 1 HA 0.77 19.24 .001
Year 1 anxiety 0.03 0.79 .43 Year 1 depression 0.01 0.28 .78
Marijuana use frequency 0.05 1.59 .11 Marijuana use frequency 0.05 1.38 .17
Gender 0.05 1.39 .17 Gender 0.05 1.53 .13
Anxiety
Marijuana Use 0.04 1.19 .23 Depression
Marijuana Use 0.02 0.43 .67
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.01 0.17 .86 Marijuana Use
Gender 0.01 0.24 .81
Anxiety
Gender 0.07 2.24 .03 Depression
Gender 0.06 1.82 .07
Anxiety
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.01 0.26 .80 Depression
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.03 0.89 .37
Novelty seeking
NS and anxiety NS and depression
Model 1: DV Anxiety t p Model 1: DV Depression t p
Year 1 anxiety 0.67 16.67 .001 Year 1 depression 0.65 14.84 .001
Year 1 NS 0.03 0.58 .56 Year 1 NS 0.05 1.18 .24
Marijuana use frequency 0.10 2.22 .03 Marijuana use frequency 0.12 2.46 .02
Gender 0.08 1.78 .08 Gender 0.04 0.76 .45
NS
Marijuana Use 0.16 3.80 .001 NS
Marijuana Use 0.13 2.93 .00
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.09 1.87 .06 Marijuana Use
Gender 0.07 1.41 .16
NS
Gender 0.05 1.26 .21 NS
Gender 0.05 1.06 .29
NS
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.04 0.91 .36 NS
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.07 1.51 .13
Model 2: DV NS t p Model 2: DV NS t p
Year 1 NS 0.77 20.61 .001 Year 1 NS 0.77 20.59 .001
Year 1 anxiety 0.06 1.80 .07 Year 1 depression 0.06 1.68 .09
Marijuana use frequency 0.03 0.91 .36 Marijuana use frequency 0.01 0.35 .73
Gender 0.05 1.31 .19 Gender 0.03 0.96 .34
Anxiety
Marijuana Use 0.01 0.37 .72 Depression
Marijuana Use 0.02 0.61 .54
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.04 1.04 .30 Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.64 .52
Anxiety
Gender 0.05 1.60 .11 Depression
Gender 0.02 0.49 .63
Anxiety
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.57 .57 Depression
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.04 1.11 .27
Reward dependence
RD and anxiety RD and depression
Model 1: DV Anxiety t p Model 1: DV Depression t p
Year 1 anxiety 0.66 16.19 .001 Year 1 depression 0.64 14.49 .001
Year 1 RD 0.04 1.08 .28 Year 1 RD 0.08 1.91 .06
Marijuana use frequency 0.01 0.14 .89 Marijuana use frequency 0.02 0.46 .65
Gender 0.12 2.97 .001 Gender 0.03 0.60 .55
RD
Marijuana Use 0.02 0.49 .62 RD
Marijuana Use 0.03 0.65 .52
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.59 .56 Marijuana Use
Gender 0.00 0.09 .93
RD
Gender 0.02 0.60 .55 RD
Gender 0.06 1.35 .18
RD
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.06 1.47 .14 RD
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.06 1.38 .17
(table continues)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MARIJUANA, TEMPERAMENT, ANXIETY, AND DEPRESSION 597
three significant predictors of Year 2 depression symptoms (see
Table 3). Year 1 depression was positively associated with Year 2
depression symptoms, and Year 1 marijuana use was negatively
associated with Year 2 depression. The NS
Marijuana Use
interaction was also significant. The simple effects in Figure 3,
Panel D show that greater Year 1 NS was associated with greater
depression in Year 2 for those with high marijuana use frequency,
.26, t(329) 3.06, p .01. By contrast, there was no relation
between NS and depression when marijuana use was low,
.03, t(329) .62, p .54.
In the second model predicting Year 2 NS, the only significant
predictor was Year 1 NS (see Table 3). Year 1 depression levels
did not predict Year 2 NS, nor did the Year 1 Depression
Marijuana Use interaction.
Reward dependence and anxiety. Figure 2, Panel E shows
the cross-lag relations of primary theoretical interest in assessing
the relation between RD and anxiety levels. The first regression
model testing whether initial temperament predicts subsequent
psychopathology revealed only two significant effects: greater
Year 1 anxiety was associated with more Year 2 anxiety and
females reported more anxiety in Year 2 than males (see Table 3).
There were no effects of RD on anxiety symptoms, either independently or moderated by marijuana use.
The second regression model predicting Year 2 RD revealed 4
significant effects (see Table 3). Greater Year 1 RD and being
female were both associated with higher Year 2 RD. There was
also an Anxiety
Marijuana Use interaction. Tests of simple
slopes showed a significant negative relation between Year 1
Table 3 (continued)
Reward dependence
RD and anxiety RD and depression
Model 2: DV RD t p Model 2: DV RD t p
Year 1 RD 0.76 22.44 .001 Year 1 RD 0.76 22.26 .001
Year 1 Anxiety 0.01 0.36 .72 Year 1 depression 0.00 0.06 .96
Marijuana use frequency 0.05 1.51 .13 Marijuana use frequency 0.05 1.56 .12
Gender 0.14 3.92 .001 Gender 0.14 3.89 .001
Anxiety
Marijuana Use 0.08 2.44 .02 Depression
Marijuana Use 0.04 0.98 .33
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.10 3.03 .01 Marijuana Use
Gender 0.10 2.86 .01
Anxiety
Gender 0.02 0.58 .56 Depression
Gender 0.02 0.51 .61
Anxiety
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.01 0.21 .83 Depression
Marijuana Use
Gender 0.02 0.62 .53
Note. All regression coefficients are standardized.
Figure 3. Simple effects of harm avoidance and novelty seeking on levels anxiety and depression as a function
of marijuana use.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
598 GRUNBERG, CORDOVA, BIDWELL, AND ITO
anxiety levels and Year 2 RD among those who more frequently
use marijuana, .14, t(329) .2.26, p .02. The relation
between Year 1 anxiety and Year 2 RD was not significant for low
frequency marijuana users, .04, t(329) 1.06, p .29.
Finally, the Marijuana Use
Gender interaction was significant.
Among women, marijuana use was positively associated with
greater RD, .15, t(329) 3.19, p .002, but the relation was
nonsignificant for men, .05, t(329) 1.08, p .28.
Reward dependence and depression. The first regression
model predicting later psychopathology from initial temperament
revealed only that Year 1 depression symptoms predicted Year 2
depression symptoms (Figure 2, Panel F). In the second model
predicting later temperament from initial psychopathology, three
effects were significant. Year 1 RD predicted Year 2 RD, and
females reported higher Year 2 RD. There was also a Marijuana
Use
Gender interaction that showed the same pattern as this
same interaction in the model predicting anxiety from RD. That is,
among women, marijuana use frequency was positively associated
with greater RD, .15, t(329) 3.05, p .002, but the relation
was nonsignificant for men, .05, t(329) .94, p .35.
Discussion
Given the elevated rates of anxiety and depression within late
adolescence and early adulthood, the present study examined how
another behavior that occurs with relatively high frequency in this
age range—marijuana use—is associated with anxiety and depression. There has to date been no clear picture of how marijuana
relates to anxiety and depression; thus, we examined how temperament may impact these relations. Of particular interest was HA,
which is associated with greater anxiety and depression. One
question that has not yet been examined is whether marijuana use
moderates the impact of the behavioral biases associated with HA
on anxiety and depression.
The temperament dimension of HA is associated with apprehension, pessimism, and inhibition and has been associated with
both anxiety and depression (Hansenne et al., 1998; Jiang et al.,
2003; Manfredi et al., 2011; Matsudaira & Kitamura, 2006). Our
prospective analyses show a similar relation, but importantly demonstrate that it is moderated by level of marijuana use. Specifically, we find that HA measured at baseline is associated with
more symptoms of both anxiety and depression measured a year
later only for those low in marijuana use. By contrast, when
marijuana use is high, HA is unrelated to anxiety and depression
levels. Of importance, these analyses control for baseline anxiety
or depression levels, so our results show the predictive effect of
HA and marijuana use over and above levels of anxiety and
depression exhibited a year earlier. While the present study cannot
speak definitively to the mechanism of our effects, marijuana has
been suggested to produce anxiolytic and mood elevating benefits
(Denson & Earleywine, 2006; Sethi et al., 1986; Stewart et al.,
1997). Such mood benefits could attenuate the greater risk toward
anxiety and depression typically associated with HA. Using our
cross-lag analysis approach, we see no evidence that initial symptoms of anxiety and depression predict subsequent HA (Model 2 in
Figure 1), suggesting that the relations observed between HA,
marijuana use, anxiety, and depression are likely due to effects of
initial temperament and marijuana use on subsequent psychopathology rather than the converse.
Marijuana use also moderated the relation between NS and
anxiety and depression symptoms, but the pattern of modulation
differed from that with HA. For NS, it was people higher in
marijuana use who showed a positive association between NS and
both anxiety and depression symptoms. Those low in marijuana
use showed no relation between NS and levels of anxiety and
depression. Further, the pattern of the interaction was such that
levels of anxiety and depression were similarly high for those high
in NS and high in marijuana use, as well as those low in NS at all
levels of marijuana use. It was individuals low in NS and high in
marijuana use that had the lowest anxiety and depression (see
Figure 3, Panels C and D). This effect warrants further investigation as it is not clear what mechanism may account for the
seemingly protective effect of low NS coupled with high marijuana use or why this benefit of marijuana use is not seen when NS
is higher. As with HA, models assessing the relation of initial
symptoms of anxiety and depression to subsequent NS fail to show
effects of anxiety, depression, or marijuana use frequency, suggesting that the relations observed between NS, marijuana use,
anxiety, and depression are likely due to effects of initial temperament and marijuana use on subsequent psychopathology rather
than the converse.
Our results reveal no effect of RD on anxiety and depression
symptoms. There were no zero-order correlations between RD and
anxiety or depression, no independent effects of RD on Year 2
anxiety or depression, and no moderated effect on anxiety and
depression. Unexpectedly, RD was the one aspect of temperament
that was predicted by an aspect of psychopathology. Specifically,
Year 1 anxiety and marijuana use frequency interacted to predict
Year 2 RD. Tests of the simple effects show that among those who
use marijuana more frequently, reporting more symptoms of anxiety in Year 1 predicted lower RD in Year 2. Temperament is
considered to be relatively stable. Consistent with this, Year 1
temperament was always a significant and large predictor of Year
2 temperament in our models, a relation that was of similar
magnitude for RD as compared with HA and NS (see Table 3). We
are, therefore, uncertain what accounts for this sole effect of initial
psychopathology predicting subsequent temperament. We interpret this relation cautiously and note that the analogous RD and
marijuana use interaction was not significant for depression symptoms.
These results have a number of implications for understanding
how marijuana affects anxiety and depression, and for how temperament affects anxiety and depression. We have noted that the
relation between marijuana and anxiety/depression has been quite
variable in past research. We think the present results demonstrate
the importance of considering the impact of other factors known to
influence anxiety and depression. The interactions we observed
between HA and marijuana use and NS and marijuana use indicate
that different relations between marijuana use and anxiety/depression levels will be observed depending on the levels of HA and NS
within a sample. Consider, for example, the interaction between
HA and marijuana use in predicting anxiety (Figure 3, Panel A). If
a sample happened to be relatively low in HA, a positive relation
between marijuana use and anxiety would be expected but if the
sample happened to be high in HA, greater marijuana use might be
associated with less anxiety. To our knowledge, no studies examining the effect of marijuana use on anxiety and depression have
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MARIJUANA, TEMPERAMENT, ANXIETY, AND DEPRESSION 599
considered how these relations interact with preexisting temperament.
It is also important to consider that the simple relations we
observed between marijuana use and depression symptoms differed from those obtained in the more complex models. That is,
when only marijuana use was considered, results suggest a
positive association between marijuana use and depression.
This was seen in the marijuana use group main effects in the
preliminary categorical analyses, and in a significant (but
small) positive bivariate correlation between Year 1 marijuana
use and Year 1 depression symptoms. However, in the regression models that prospectively predict anxiety/depression and
also include HA or RD, the HA
Marijuana Use or RD
Marijuana Use interactions, and baseline anxiety or depression,
marijuana use was not an independent predictor of depression
symptoms. Moreover, in the models involving NS, marijuana
use negatively predicted depression symptoms (and anxiety).
These differing patterns of results first demonstrate the importance of measuring the effects of marijuana within the context
of other factors known to affect anxiety and depression, as well
as prior symptoms of anxiety and depression. The results might
also indicate a complex causal relation between marijuana use
and depression in which initial symptoms of depression facilitate marijuana use, which subsequently decreases depression.
This conclusion, however, is speculative and warrants more
explicit examination. At a minimum, our results suggest marijuana does not have an invariant effect of anxiety and depression, and that more research is needed to understand the possible mechanisms through which aspects of temperament and
marijuana use affects anxiety and depression.
Another intriguing question raised by our results is whether
temperament could affect marijuana use. HA is associated with
negative affective states such as apprehension and pessimism, as
well as social inhibition (Cloninger et al., 1993). Coping with
negative affect, enhancing positive affect, and facilitating social
interactions are among the top motivations reported for marijuana
use (Lee et al., 2009; Newcomb et al., 1988; Simons et al., 1998),
raising the possibility that people high in HA might use marijuana
as a way to manage their temperamental predispositions. As marijuana use increases, studying not only the outcomes of its use but
also the factors that motivate its use is of increasing relevance. Our
results suggest additional specific relations that might be fruitful to
examine.
Effects of Gender
Roughly equal numbers of men and women with similar rates of
marijuana use were recruited in this sample, providing the opportunity to assess whether predictors of level of anxiety and depression in this age range differ for men and women. There were some
gender main effects in the regression analyses predicting anxiety,
with women reporting more symptoms of anxiety in Year 2 than
men, even when controlling for Year 1 anxiety. This result indicates that anxiety increased more for women that for men in this
sample, broadly consistent with higher prevalence rates of anxiety
among women (e.g., McLean et al., 2011; National Institute of
Mental Health, 2014a). However, gender did not moderate any of
the effects of temperament, or the Temperament
Marijuana Use
interactions, suggesting that the way temperament relates to anxiety and depression—and how that relation is moderated by marijuana use— occurs similarly for men and women.
Limitations
While our prospective design, relatively large sample, and
roughly equal numbers of men and women were strengths of our
design, there are limitations to consider. We purposely focused our
analyses on a college sample because of the high clinical significance of anxiety, depression, and marijuana use in this age range.
However, it is important to interpret results with these contextual
factors in mind; it is possible that the relation among these variables differs in other types of samples.
Another factor to consider is our use of self-reported symptoms
of anxiety and depression. Although the measure we used to assess
anxiety and depression has demonstrated validity with clinical
assessments (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003), no independent clinical assessments were available for our participants. Moreover, we
examined our hypotheses within the context of a broad range of
anxiety and depression symptomology. While the number of participants meeting clinical thresholds for anxiety and depression
was modest (see Table 1), subclinical levels of anxiety and depression are associated with meaningful functional impairments
(Dotson et al., 2014; Karsten, Nolen, Penninx, & Hartman, 2011;
Kessler, Zhao, Blazer, & Swartz, 1997). Subclinical symptoms are
also precursors to clinical conditions (Hill, Pettit, Lewinsohn,
Seeley, & Klein, 2014; Shankman et al., 2009).
A final important consideration is that our analyses are based on
correlational data. Given the variables involved—temperament,
substance use, and symptoms associated with psychopathology—
true experiments are not feasible. We attempted to maximize our
inferential power through methodological decisions such as the
longitudinal design so that marijuana use and temperament were
measured 1 year before anxiety and depression symptoms. Use of
cross-lag analyses also allowed us to statically assess the plausibility of our proposed model that initial temperament and marijuana use affect subsequent and depression over the alternative
model that initial anxiety/depression and marijuana use affect
subsequent temperament. Nevertheless, our conclusions are ultimately correlational and the implications of the results must be
interpreted with that limitation in mind. Moreover, that conclusions are based on only two points in time is another limitation to
consider.
Conclusions
Our results highlight the importance of simultaneously assessing
the effect of marijuana use on anxiety and depression within the
context of other factors known to predict psychopathology by
showing that marijuana use and temperament interact complexly in
predicting risk for anxiety and depression. These results may help
explain why marijuana’s effects on anxiety and depression have
been variable in past research. They also show that even for a
temperament dimension consistently linked with anxiety and depression (HA), other factors have important moderating influences
on its effects. Another important implication of our results is that
it is not only important to assess these complex relations among
multiple factors, but also the causal mechanisms they imply. Our
results suggest that marijuana’s anxiolytic and mood-enhancing
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
600 GRUNBERG, CORDOVA, BIDWELL, AND ITO
effects (or perceived effects) may attenuate the effects of HA on
risk for anxiety and depression, raising the question of whether
such effects might motivate individuals high in HA to use marijuana. As marijuana use becomes more readily available and
accepted, it will be important to consider these kinds of potential
motivations for its use. Finally, from the perspective of treatment
and intervention, the lack of simple effects of marijuana use on
anxiety and depression argues against marijuana reduction interventions as strategies that will necessarily decrease risk for anxiety
and depression within this age range. Instead, the benefits of
decreasing marijuana use will depend on other characteristics, such
as temperament.
References
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult
forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research
Center for Children, Youth, and Families.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Barkley, R., & Murphy, K. (1998). A clinical workbook: Attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
51, 1173–1182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory
for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893– 897. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties
of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation.
Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0272-
7358(88)90050-5
Bonn-Miller, M. O., Zvolensky, M. J., Leen-Feldner, E. W., Feldner,
M. T., & Yartz, A. R. (2005). Marijuana use among daily tobacco
smokers: Relationship to anxiety-related factors. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 279 –289. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10862-005-2408-6
Carey, K. B., Neal, D. J., & Collins, S. E. (2004). A psychometric analysis
of the self-regulation questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 253–260.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.001
Chen, C.-Y., Wagner, F. A., & Anthony, J. C. (2002). Marijuana use and
the risk of Major Depressive Episode. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology, 37, 199 –206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-002-
0541-z
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 50, 975–990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993
.01820240059008
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (2006). Can personality assessment predict future depression? A twelve-month follow-up of
631 subjects. Journal of Affective Disorders, 92, 35– 44. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.034
CNN Gallup. (2013, August 2). U.S.A. Today Poll. Retrieved from http://
www.gallup.com/poll/163835/tried-marijuana-little-changed-80s.aspx
Colorado Amendment 64. (2012). Amendment 64: Use and regulation of
marijuana. Retrieved from http://www.colorado.gov/cs/Satellite?blobcol
urldata&blobheaderapplication%2Fpdf&blobkeyid&blobtable
MungoBlobs&blobwhere1251834064719&ssbinarytrue
Copeland, W., Landry, K., Stanger, C., & Hudziak, J. J. (2004). Multiinformant assessment of temperament in children with externalizing
behavior problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 547–556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3303_12
de Paula Soares, V., Campos, A. C., Bortoli, V. C., Zangrossi, H., Jr.,
Guimarães, F. S., & Zuardi, A. W. (2010). Intra-dorsal periaqueductal
gray administration of cannabidiol blocks panic-like response by activating 5-HT1A receptors. Behavioural Brain Research, 213, 225–229.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.004
Degenhardt, L., Chiu, W.-T., Sampson, N., Kessler, R. C., Anthony, J. C.,
Angermeyer, M.,… Wells, J. E. (2008). Toward a global view of
alcohol, tobacco, cannabis, and cocaine use: Findings from the WHO
World Mental Health Surveys. PLoS Medicine, 5, e141. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0050141
Degenhardt, L., Hall, W., & Lynskey, M. (2001). The relationship between
cannabis use, depression and anxiety among Australian adults: Findings
from the National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being. Social
Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36, 219 –227. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/s001270170052
Degenhardt, L., Hall, W., & Lynskey, M. (2003). Exploring the association
between cannabis use and depression. Addiction, 98, 1493–1504. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00437.x
Denson, T. F., & Earleywine, M. (2006). Decreased depression in marijuana users. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 738 –742. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.052
Dotson, V. M., Szymkowicz, S. M., Kirton, J. W., McLaren, M. E., Green,
M. L., & Rohani, J. Y. (2014). Unique and interactive effect of anxiety
and depressive symptoms on cognitive and brain function in young and
older adults. Journal of Depression and Anxiety, S1, 1–10. http://dx.doi
.org/10.4172/2167-1044.S1-003
Farmer, A., Mahmood, A., Redman, K., Harris, T., Sadler, S., & McGuffin,
P. (2003). A sib-pair study of the Temperament and Character Inventory
scales in major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 490 –
496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.490
Green, B. E., & Ritter, C. (2000). Marijuana use and depression. Journal
of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 40 – 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
2676359
Guimarães, F. S., Chiaretti, T. M., Graeff, F. G., & Zuardi, A. W. (1990).
Antianxiety effect of cannabidiol in the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology, 100, 558 –559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02244012
Hansenne, M., Pitchot, W., Gonzalez Moreno, A., Machurot, P. Y., &
Ansseau, M. (1998). The tridimensional personality questionnaire (TPQ)
and depression. European Psychiatry, 13, 101–103. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0924-9338(98)80026-6
Hansenne, M., Reggers, J., Pinto, E., Kjiri, K., Ajamier, A., & Ansseau, M.
(1999). Temperament and character inventory (TCI) and depression.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33, 31–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0022-3956(98)00036-3
Hayatbakhsh, M. R., Najman, J. M., Jamrozik, K., Mamun, A. A., Alati, R.,
& Bor, W. (2007). Cannabis and anxiety and depression in young adults:
A large prospective study. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 408 – 417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/chi
.0b013e31802dc54d
Hill, R. M., Pettit, J. W., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Klein, D. N.
(2014). Escalation to Major Depressive Disorder among adolescents
with subthreshold depressive symptoms: Evidence of distinct subgroups
at risk. Journal of Affective Disorders, 158, 133–138. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.011
Jiang, N., Sato, T., Hara, T., Takedomi, Y., Ozaki, I., & Yamada, S. (2003).
Correlations between trait anxiety, personality and fatigue. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 55, 493–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0022-3999(03)00021-7
Karsten, J., Nolen, W. A., Penninx, B. W., & Hartman, C. A. (2011).
Subthreshold anxiety better defined by symptom self-report than by
diagnostic interview. Journal of Affective Disorders, 129, 236 –243.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.09.006
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., &
Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MARIJUANA, TEMPERAMENT, ANXIETY, AND DEPRESSION 601
of DSM–IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593– 602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/
archpsyc.62.6.593
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Swartz, M., Blazer, D. G., & Nelson,
C. B. (1993). Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I:
Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 29, 85–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-G
Kessler, R. C., Zhao, S., Blazer, D. G., & Swartz, M. (1997). Prevalence,
correlates, and course of minor depression and major depression in the
National Comorbidity Survey. Journal of Affective Disorders, 45, 19 –
30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00056-6
Lee, C. M., Neighbors, C., Hendershot, C. S., & Grossbard, J. R. (2009).
Development and preliminary validation of a comprehensive marijuana
motives questionnaire. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 70,
279 –287. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2009.70.279
Manfredi, C., Caselli, G., Rovetto, F., Rebecchi, D., Ruggiero, G. M.,
Sassaroli, S., & Spada, M. M. (2011). Temperament and parental styles
as predictors of ruminative brooding and worry. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 186 –191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010
.09.023
Matsudaira, T., & Kitamura, T. (2006). Personality traits as risk factors of
depression and anxiety among Japanese students. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 62, 97–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20215
McLean, C. P., Asnaani, A., Litz, B. T., & Hofmann, S. G. (2011). Gender
differences in anxiety disorders: Prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 1027–
1035. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.006
Mechoulam, R., Parker, L. A., & Gallily, R. (2002). Cannabidiol: An
overview of some pharmacological aspects. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 42, 11S–19S. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.1552-4604.2002
.tb05998.x
Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S.,
Cui, L.,… Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey
Replication—Adolescent Suppl. (NCS-A). Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 980 –989. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017
Musty, R. E., & Kaback, L. (1995). Relationships between motivation and
depression in chronic marijuana users. Life Sciences, 56, 2151–2158.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(95)00202-H
National Institute of Drug Abuse. (2014). DrugFacts: Marijuana. Retrieved
from http://www.drugabuse.gov/publications/drugfacts/marijuana
National Institute of Mental Health. (2014a). Anxiety disorders. Retrieved
from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/anxiety-disorders/index
.shtml
National Institute of Mental Health. (2014b). Any anxiety disorder among
adults. Retrieved fromhttp://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/prevalence/
any-anxiety-disorder-among-adults.shtml
National Institute of Mental Health. (2014c). Any mood disorder among
adults. Retrieved fromhttp://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/prevalence/
any-mood-disorder-among-adults.shtml
Newcomb, M. D., Chou, C.-P., Bentler, P. M., & Huba, G. (1988).
Cognitive motivations for drug use among adolescents: Longitudinal
tests of gender differences and predictors of change in drug use. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 35, 426 – 438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-0167.35.4.426
Rogosa, D. (1980). A critique of cross-lagged correlation. Psychological
Bulletin, 88, 245–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.2.245
Scholes-Balog, K. E., Hemphill, S. A., Patton, G. C., & Toumbourou, J. W.
(2013). Cannabis use and related harms in the transition to young
adulthood: A longitudinal study of Australian secondary school students.
Journal of Adolescence, 36, 519 –527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.adolescence.2013.03.001
Sethi, B. B., Trivedi, J. K., Kumar, P., Gulati, A., Agarwal, A. K., & Sethi,
N. (1986). Antianxiety effect of cannabis: Involvement of central benzodiazepine receptors. Biological Psychiatry, 21, 3–10. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/0006-3223(86)90003-X
Shankman, S. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., Small, J. W., Seeley,
J. R., & Altman, S. E. (2009). Subthreshold conditions as precursors for
full syndrome disorders: A 15-year longitudinal study of multiple diagnostic classes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1485–
1494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02117.x
Simons, J., Correia, C. J., Carey, K. B., & Borsari, B. E. (1998). Validating
a five-factor marijuana motives measure: Relations with use, problems,
and alcohol motives. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 265–273.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.45.3.265
Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1992). Timeline follow-back: A technique
for assessing self-reported ethanol consumption. In J. Allen & R. Z.
Litten (Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biological methods (pp. 41–72). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-0357-5_3
Starcevic, V., Uhlenhuth, E. H., Fallon, S., & Pathak, D. (1996). Personality dimensions in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 37, 75–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
0165-0327(95)00058-5
Stewart, S. H., Karp, J., Pihl, R. O., & Peterson, R. A. (1997). Anxiety
sensitivity and self-reported reasons for drug use. Journal of Substance
Abuse, 9, 223–240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0899-3289(97)90018-3
Strakowski, S. M., Dunayevich, E., Keck, P. E., Jr., & McElroy, S. L.
(1995). Affective state dependence of the Tridimensional Personality
Questionnaire. Psychiatry Research, 57, 209 –214. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/0165-1781(95)02655-G
Washington Initiative 502. (2012). Summary of Initiative 502. Retrieved from
http://www.leg.wa.gov/House/Committees/OPRGeneral/Documents/2012/
I-502%20summary.pdf
Young, L. T., Bagby, R. M., Cooke, R. G., Parker, J. D., Levitt, A. J., &
Joffe, R. T. (1995). A comparison of Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire dimensions in bipolar disorder and unipolar depression. Psychiatry Research, 58, 139 –143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-
1781(95)02684-O
Zuardi, A. W., Crippa, J. A., Hallak, J. E., Moreira, F. A., & Guimarães,
F. S. (2006). Cannabidiol, a Cannabis sativa constituent, as an antipsychotic drug. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 39,
421– 429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2006000400001
Received January 13, 2015
Revision received May 26, 2015
Accepted June 8, 2015
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
602 GRUNBERG, CORDOVA, BIDWELL, AND ITO
Get Professional Assignment Help Cheaply
Are you busy and do not have time to handle your assignment? Are you scared that your paper will not make the grade? Do you have responsibilities that may hinder you from turning in your assignment on time? Are you tired and can barely handle your assignment? Are your grades inconsistent?
Whichever your reason is, it is valid! You can get professional academic help from our service at affordable rates. We have a team of professional academic writers who can handle all your assignments.
Why Choose Our Academic Writing Service?
- Plagiarism free papers
- Timely delivery
- Any deadline
- Skilled, Experienced Native English Writers
- Subject-relevant academic writer
- Adherence to paper instructions
- Ability to tackle bulk assignments
- Reasonable prices
- 24/7 Customer Support
- Get superb grades consistently
Online Academic Help With Different Subjects
Literature
Students barely have time to read. We got you! Have your literature essay or book review written without having the hassle of reading the book. You can get your literature paper custom-written for you by our literature specialists.
Finance
Do you struggle with finance? No need to torture yourself if finance is not your cup of tea. You can order your finance paper from our academic writing service and get 100% original work from competent finance experts.
Computer science
Computer science is a tough subject. Fortunately, our computer science experts are up to the match. No need to stress and have sleepless nights. Our academic writers will tackle all your computer science assignments and deliver them on time. Let us handle all your python, java, ruby, JavaScript, php , C+ assignments!
Psychology
While psychology may be an interesting subject, you may lack sufficient time to handle your assignments. Don’t despair; by using our academic writing service, you can be assured of perfect grades. Moreover, your grades will be consistent.
Engineering
Engineering is quite a demanding subject. Students face a lot of pressure and barely have enough time to do what they love to do. Our academic writing service got you covered! Our engineering specialists follow the paper instructions and ensure timely delivery of the paper.
Nursing
In the nursing course, you may have difficulties with literature reviews, annotated bibliographies, critical essays, and other assignments. Our nursing assignment writers will offer you professional nursing paper help at low prices.
Sociology
Truth be told, sociology papers can be quite exhausting. Our academic writing service relieves you of fatigue, pressure, and stress. You can relax and have peace of mind as our academic writers handle your sociology assignment.
Business
We take pride in having some of the best business writers in the industry. Our business writers have a lot of experience in the field. They are reliable, and you can be assured of a high-grade paper. They are able to handle business papers of any subject, length, deadline, and difficulty!
Statistics
We boast of having some of the most experienced statistics experts in the industry. Our statistics experts have diverse skills, expertise, and knowledge to handle any kind of assignment. They have access to all kinds of software to get your assignment done.
Law
Writing a law essay may prove to be an insurmountable obstacle, especially when you need to know the peculiarities of the legislative framework. Take advantage of our top-notch law specialists and get superb grades and 100% satisfaction.
What discipline/subjects do you deal in?
We have highlighted some of the most popular subjects we handle above. Those are just a tip of the iceberg. We deal in all academic disciplines since our writers are as diverse. They have been drawn from across all disciplines, and orders are assigned to those writers believed to be the best in the field. In a nutshell, there is no task we cannot handle; all you need to do is place your order with us. As long as your instructions are clear, just trust we shall deliver irrespective of the discipline.
Are your writers competent enough to handle my paper?
Our essay writers are graduates with bachelor's, masters, Ph.D., and doctorate degrees in various subjects. The minimum requirement to be an essay writer with our essay writing service is to have a college degree. All our academic writers have a minimum of two years of academic writing. We have a stringent recruitment process to ensure that we get only the most competent essay writers in the industry. We also ensure that the writers are handsomely compensated for their value. The majority of our writers are native English speakers. As such, the fluency of language and grammar is impeccable.
What if I don’t like the paper?
There is a very low likelihood that you won’t like the paper.
Reasons being:
- When assigning your order, we match the paper’s discipline with the writer’s field/specialization. Since all our writers are graduates, we match the paper’s subject with the field the writer studied. For instance, if it’s a nursing paper, only a nursing graduate and writer will handle it. Furthermore, all our writers have academic writing experience and top-notch research skills.
- We have a quality assurance that reviews the paper before it gets to you. As such, we ensure that you get a paper that meets the required standard and will most definitely make the grade.
In the event that you don’t like your paper:
- The writer will revise the paper up to your pleasing. You have unlimited revisions. You simply need to highlight what specifically you don’t like about the paper, and the writer will make the amendments. The paper will be revised until you are satisfied. Revisions are free of charge
- We will have a different writer write the paper from scratch.
- Last resort, if the above does not work, we will refund your money.
Will the professor find out I didn’t write the paper myself?
Not at all. All papers are written from scratch. There is no way your tutor or instructor will realize that you did not write the paper yourself. In fact, we recommend using our assignment help services for consistent results.
What if the paper is plagiarized?
We check all papers for plagiarism before we submit them. We use powerful plagiarism checking software such as SafeAssign, LopesWrite, and Turnitin. We also upload the plagiarism report so that you can review it. We understand that plagiarism is academic suicide. We would not take the risk of submitting plagiarized work and jeopardize your academic journey. Furthermore, we do not sell or use prewritten papers, and each paper is written from scratch.
When will I get my paper?
You determine when you get the paper by setting the deadline when placing the order. All papers are delivered within the deadline. We are well aware that we operate in a time-sensitive industry. As such, we have laid out strategies to ensure that the client receives the paper on time and they never miss the deadline. We understand that papers that are submitted late have some points deducted. We do not want you to miss any points due to late submission. We work on beating deadlines by huge margins in order to ensure that you have ample time to review the paper before you submit it.
Will anyone find out that I used your services?
We have a privacy and confidentiality policy that guides our work. We NEVER share any customer information with third parties. Noone will ever know that you used our assignment help services. It’s only between you and us. We are bound by our policies to protect the customer’s identity and information. All your information, such as your names, phone number, email, order information, and so on, are protected. We have robust security systems that ensure that your data is protected. Hacking our systems is close to impossible, and it has never happened.
How our Assignment Help Service Works
1. Place an order
You fill all the paper instructions in the order form. Make sure you include all the helpful materials so that our academic writers can deliver the perfect paper. It will also help to eliminate unnecessary revisions.
2. Pay for the order
Proceed to pay for the paper so that it can be assigned to one of our expert academic writers. The paper subject is matched with the writer’s area of specialization.
3. Track the progress
You communicate with the writer and know about the progress of the paper. The client can ask the writer for drafts of the paper. The client can upload extra material and include additional instructions from the lecturer. Receive a paper.
4. Download the paper
The paper is sent to your email and uploaded to your personal account. You also get a plagiarism report attached to your paper.
PLACE THIS ORDER OR A SIMILAR ORDER WITH US TODAY AND GET A PERFECT SCORE!!!
